Ãåíäåðíûé ìîçã. Ñîâðåìåííàÿ íåéðîáèîëîãèÿ ðàçâåí÷èâàåò ìèô î æåíñêîì ìîçãå  - ÷èòàòü îíëàéí êíèãó. Àâòîð: Äæèíà Ðèïïîí còð.¹ 104

÷èòàòü êíèãè îíëàéí áåñïëàòíî
 
 

Îíëàéí êíèãà - Ãåíäåðíûé ìîçã. Ñîâðåìåííàÿ íåéðîáèîëîãèÿ ðàçâåí÷èâàåò ìèô î æåíñêîì ìîçãå  | Àâòîð êíèãè - Äæèíà Ðèïïîí

Còðàíèöà 104
÷èòàòü îíëàéí êíèãè áåñïëàòíî

Ãëàâà 6

Ñîöèàëüíûé ìîçã

1. M. D. Lieberman, Social: Why Our Brains Are Wired to Connect (Oxford, Oxford University Press, 2013). • 2. R. Adolphs, ‘Investigating the Cognitive Neuroscience of Human Social Behavior’, Neuropsychologia 41:2 (2003), pp. 119–26; D. M. Amodio, E. Harman-Jones, P. G. Devine, J. J. Curtin, S. L. Hartley and A. E. Covert, ‘Neural Signals for the Detection of Unintentional Race Bias’, Psychological Science 15:2 (2004), pp. 88–93. • 3. D. I. Tamir and M. A. Thornton, ‘Modeling the Predictive Social Mind’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 22:3 (2018), pp. 201–12; P. Hinton, ‘Implicit Stereotypes and the Predictive Brain: Cognition and Culture in “Biased” Person Perception’, Palgrave Communications 3 (2017), 17086. • 4. Frith and Frith, ‘The Social Brain: Allowing Humans to Boldly Go Where No Other Species Has Been’, pp. 165–76. • 5. P. Adjamian, A. Hadjipapas, G. R. Barnes, A. Hillebrand and I. E. Holliday, ‘Induced Gamma Activity in Primary Visual Cortex Is Related to Luminance and Not Color Contrast: An MEG Study’, Journal of Vision 8:7 (2008), art. 4. • 6. M. V. Lombardo, J. L. Barnes, S. J. Wheelwright and S. Baron-Cohen, ‘Self-Referential Cognition and Empathy in Autism’, PLoS One 2:9 (2007), e883. • 7. T. Singer, ‘The Neuronal Basis and Ontogeny of Empathy and Mind Reading: Review of Literature and Implications for Future Research’, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 30:6 (2006), pp. 855–63. • 8. C. D. Frith, ‘The Social Brain?’, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362:1480 (2007), pp. 671–8. • 9. R. Adolphs, D. Tranel and A. R. Damasio, ‘The Human Amygdala in Social Judgment’, Nature 393:6684 (1998), p. 470. • 10. A. J. Hart, P. J. Whalen, L. M. Shin, S. C. McInerney, H. Fischer and S. L. Rauch, ‘Differential Response in the Human Amygdala to Racial Outgroup vs Ingroup Face Stimuli’, Neuroreport 11:11 (2000), pp. 2351–4. • 11. D. M. Amodio and C. D. Frith, ‘Meeting of Minds: The Medial Frontal Cortex and Social Cognition’, Nature Reviews Neuroscience 7:4 (2006), p. 268. • 12. Òàì æå. • 13. Òàì æå. • 14. S. J. Gillihan and M. J. Farah, ‘Is Self Special? A Critical Review of Evidence from Experimental Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience’, Psychological Bulletin 131:1 (2005), p. 76. • 15. D. A. Gusnard, E. Akbudak, G. L. Shulman and M. E. Raichle, ‘Medial Prefrontal Cortex and Self-Referential Mental Activity: Relation to a Default Mode of Brain Function’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 98:7 (2001), pp. 4259–64; R. B. Mars, F. X. Neubert, M. P. Noonan, J. Sallet, I. Toni and M. F. Rushworth, ‘On the Relationship between the “Default Mode Network” and the “Social Brain”’, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6 (2012), p. 189. • 16. N. I. Eisenberger, M. D. Lieberman and K. D. Williams, ‘Does Rejection Hurt? An fMRI Study of Social Exclusion’, Science 302:5643 (2003), pp. 290–92. • 17. N. I. Eisenberger, T. K. Inagaki, K. A. Muscatell, K. E. Byrne Haltom and M. R. Leary, ‘The Neural Sociometer: Brain Mechanisms Underlying State Self-Esteem’, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 23:11 (2011), pp. 3448–55. • 18. L. H. Somerville, T. F. Heatherton and W. M. Kelley, ‘Anterior Cingulate Cortex Responds Differentially to Expectancy Violation and Social Rejection’, Nature Neuroscience 9:8 (2006), p. 1007. • 19. T. Dalgleish, N. D. Walsh, D. Mobbs, S. Schweizer, A-L. van Harmelen, B. Dunn, V. Dunn, I. Goodyer and J. Stretton, ‘Social Pain and Social Gain in the Adolescent Brain: A Common Neural Circuitry Underlying Both Positive and Negative Social Evaluation’, Scientific Reports 7 (2017), 42010. • 20. N. I. Eisenberger and M. D. Lieberman, ‘Why Rejection Hurts: A Common Neural Alarm System for Physical and Social Pain’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8:7 (2004), pp. 294–300. • 21. M. R. Leary, E. S. Tambor, S. K. Terdal and D. L. Downs, ‘Self-Esteem as an Interpersonal Monitor: The Sociometer Hypothesis’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 68:3 (1995), p. 518. • 22. M. M. Botvinick, J. D. Cohen and C. S. Carter, ‘Conflict Monitoring and Anterior Cingulate Cortex: An Update’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8:12 (2004), pp. 539–46. • 23. Botvinick et al., ‘Conflict Monitoring and Anterior Cingulate Cortex’. • 24. A. D. Craig, ‘How Do You Feel – Now? The Anterior Insula and Human Awareness’, Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10:1 (2009), pp. 59–70. • 25. Òàì æå. • 26. Eisenberger et al., ‘The Neural Sociometer’. • 27. K. Onoda, Y. Okamoto, K. I. Nakashima, H. Nittono, S. Yoshimura, S. Yamawaki, S. Yamaguchi and M. Ura, ‘Does Low Self-Esteem Enhance Social Pain? The Relationship between Trait Self-Esteem and Anterior Cingulate Cortex Activation Induced by Ostracism’, Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 5:4 (2010), pp. 385–91. • 28. J. P. Bhanji and M. R. Delgado, ‘The Social Brain and Reward: Social Information Processing in the Human Striatum’, Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 5:1 (2014), pp. 61–73. • 29. S. Bray and J. O’Doherty, ‘Neural Coding of Reward-Prediction Error Signals during Classical Conditioning with Attractive Faces’, Journal of Neurophysiology 97:4 (2007), pp. 3036–45. • 30. D. A. Hackman and M. J. Farah, ‘Socioeconomic Status and the Developing Brain’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13:2 (2009), pp. 65–73. • 31. P. J. Gianaros, J. A. Horenstein, S. Cohen, K. A. Matthews, S. M. Brown, J. D. Flory, H. D. Critchley, S. B. Manuck and A. R. Hariri, ‘Perigenual Anterior Cingulate Morphology Covaries with Perceived Social Standing’, Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 2:3 (2007), pp. 161–73. • 32. O. Longe, F. A. Maratos, P. Gilbert, G. Evans, F. Volker, H. Rockliff and G. Rippon, ‘Having a Word with Yourself: Neural Correlates of Self-Criticism and Self-Reassurance’, NeuroImage 49:2 (2010), pp. 1849–56. • 33. B. T. Denny, H. Kober, T. D. Wager and K. N. Ochsner, ‘A Meta-analysis of Functional Neuroimaging Studies of Self- and Other Judgments Reveals a Spatial Gradient for Mentalizing in Medial Prefrontal Cortex’, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 24:8 (2012), pp. 1742–52. • 34. H. Tajfel, ‘Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations’, Annual Review of Psychology 33 (1982), pp. 1–39. • 35. P. Molenberghs, ‘The Neuroscience of In-Group Bias’, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37:8 (2013), pp. 1530–36. • 36. J. K. Rilling, J. E. Dagenais, D. R. Goldsmith, A. L. Glenn and G. Pagnoni, ‘Social Cognitive Neural Networks during In-Group and Out-Group Interactions’, NeuroImage 41:4 (2008), pp. 1447–61. • 37. C. Frith and U. Frith, ‘Theory of Mind’, Current Biology 15:17 (2005), pp. R644–5; D. Premack and G. Woodruff, ‘Does the Chimpanzee Have a Theory of Mind?’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1:4 (1978), pp. 515–26. • 38. Amodio and Frith, ‘Meeting of Minds’. • 39. V. Gallese and A. Goldman, ‘Mirror Neurons and the Simulation Theory of Mind-Reading’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2:12 (1998), pp. 493–501. • 40. M. Schulte-Rüther, H. J. Markowitsch, G. R. Fink and M. Pief ke, ‘Mirror Neuron and Theory of Mind Mechanisms Involved in Face-to-Face Interactions: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Approach to Empathy’, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19:8 (2007), pp. 1354–72. • 41. S. G. Shamay-Tsoory, J. Aharon-Peretz and D. Perry, ‘Two Systems for Empathy: A Double Dissociation between Emotional and Cognitive Empathy in Inferior Frontal Gyrus versus Ventromedial Prefrontal Lesions’, Brain 132:3 (2009), pp. 617–27. • 42. M. Iacoboni and J. C. Mazziotta, ‘Mirror Neuron System: Basic Findings and Clinical Applications’, Annals of Neurology 62:3 (2007), pp. 213–18; M. Iacoboni, ‘Imitation, Empathy, and Mirror Neurons’, Annual Review of Psychology 60 (2009), pp. 653–70. • 43. J. M. Contreras, M. R. Banaji and J. P. Mitchell, ‘Dissociable Neural Correlates of Stereotypes and Other Forms of Semantic Knowledge’, Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 7:7 (2011), pp. 764–70. • 44. S. J. Spencer, C. M. Steele and D. M. Quinn, ‘Stereotype Threat and Women’s Math Performance’, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 35:1 (1999), pp. 4–28; T. Schmader, ‘Gender Identification Moderates Stereotype Threat Effects on Women’s Math Performance’, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 38:2 (2002), pp. 194–201. • 45. T. Schmader, M. Johns and C. Forbes, ‘An Integrated Process Model of Stereotype Threat Effects on Performance’, Psychological Review 115:2 (2008), p. 336. • 46. M. Wraga, M. Helt, E. Jacobs and K. Sullivan, ‘Neural Basis of Stereotype-Induced Shifts in Women’s Mental Rotation Performance’, Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 2:1 (2007), pp. 12–19. • 47. M. Wraga, L. Duncan, E. C. Jacobs, M. Helt and J. Church, ‘Stereotype Susceptibility Narrows the Gender Gap in Imagined Self-Rotation Performance’, Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 13:5 (2006), pp. 813–19. • 48. Wraga et al., ‘Neural Basis of Stereotype-Induced Shifts’. • 49. H. J. Spiers, B. C. Love, M. E. Le Pelley, C. E. Gibb and R. A. Murphy, ‘Anterior Temporal Lobe Tracks the Formation of Prejudice’, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 29:3 (2017), pp. 530–44; R. I. Dunbar, ‘The Social Brain Hypothesis’, Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews 6:5 (1998), pp. 178–90. • 50. Dunbar, ‘The Social Brain Hypothesis’. • 51. J. Stiles, ‘Neural Plasticity and Cognitive Development’, Developmental Neuropsychology 18:2 (2000), pp. 237–72.

Âåðíóòüñÿ ê ïðîñìîòðó êíèãè Ïåðåéòè ê Îãëàâëåíèþ Ïåðåéòè ê Ïðèìå÷àíèþ